• Skip to primary navigation
  • Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar

Lisa Feldman Barrett

Neuroscientist, Psychologist, and Author

  • About
    • Biography
    • Quick Introduction
  • Books
    • How Emotions are Made
    • Seven and a Half Lessons About the Brain
    • Other Books
    • British Editions
    • كتب عربية (Arabic)
    • български (Bulgarian)
    • 简体中文书籍 (Chinese, Simplified)
    • 繁體中文書籍 (Chinese, Traditional)
    • Hrvatske knjige (Croatian)
    • České knihy (Czech)
    • Eesti raamatud (Estonian)
    • Suomalaisia kirjoja (Finnish)
    • Deutsche Bücher (German)
    • Ελληνικά βιβλία (Greek)
    • Libri in italiano (Italian)
    • 日本の本 (Japanese)
    • 한국어로 된 책들 (Korean)
    • Polskie książki (Polish)
    • Livros portugueses (Portuguese)
    • Cărți românești (Romanian)
    • Русские Книги (Russian)
    • Slovenské knihy (Slovak)
    • Slovenske knjige (Slovenian)
    • Libros en español (Spanish)
    • Svenska böcker (Swedish)
    • หนังสือภาษาไทย (Thai)
    • བོད་ཡིག་དཔེ་དེབ། (Tibetan)
    • Türkçe kitaplar (Turkish)
    • Українські книги (Ukrainian)
  • Articles
    • Popular
    • APS Column
    • Academic
  • Watch/Listen
    • TED Talk
    • Videos
    • Podcasts
  • Press
  • Events
  • Blog
  • Contact
    • Contact me
    • LinkedIn
    • BlueSky
    • TikTok
    • X/Twitter
    • YouTube

Animals, Emotion, Mental Inference Fallacy, Myths About the Brain, Psychology, Vocabulary · May 24, 2017

The Mental Inference Fallacy

Dr. Frankenstein was up late one night, working on his monster project. He popped open the monster’s skull, attached electrodes to the brain in a random spot, and zapped it with electric current. The monster’s leg gave a little kick. “Hmm,” said Dr. Frankenstein. “Those neurons appear to control a kicking reflex.”

The good doctor moved the electrodes and delivered another jolt. This time, the monster’s pinkies wiggled. “Interesting,” remarked Dr. Frankenstein. “Circuitry for finger movement. Perhaps my monster will be capable of playing the piano.”

Finally, he placed the electrodes in a third spot and applied the current. This time, the corners of the monster’s mouth curled upward. “Eureka!” cried Dr. Frankenstein. “I’ve discovered the brain circuit for happiness!”

What just happened here? Dr. Frankenstein observed three different bodily motions in response to his electrical probe. He perceived the first two as simple actions but the third one as emotional. That’s because in his culture, an upturned mouth (i.e., a smile) has a meaning to communicate happiness.

In my book How Emotions are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain, I present evidence that the brain has no circuitry dedicated to any specific emotion, such as happiness, despite what Dr. Frankenstein might think. Plenty of other scientists, however, have claimed to locate distinct brain circuits for individual emotions, even in animals as diverse as monkeys, rats, crayfish, and flies. If that’s the case, how can there be no brain circuits for emotion?

The solution is simple but not obvious. Scientists who claim to find circuits for emotion in animal brains are doing what Dr. Frankenstein did. They’re discovering circuits for actions — running, teeth-baring, freezing in place, and so forth. But instead of recognizing these actions as collections of movements, the scientists perceive them as emotional, and write about them as emotional. Objectively speaking, these actions have no inherent emotional meaning. Freezing does not equal fear. Teeth-baring does not equal anger.

I call this error the mental inference fallacy. It means mistaking an action for an emotion in a scientific setting.

An action like smiling is a coordinated collection of movements. An emotion such as happiness is much more complex. It might involve smiling or it might not. It could involve other actions such as jumping, fist-bumping, crying, or even standing still with eyes closed. In addition, your culture has rules about when happiness is appropriate and when it isn’t. In short, you have many diverse concepts relating to happiness: its meaning, its causes, and its consequences. In the blink of an eye, your brain applies these concepts to construct meaning from the actions of others.

In exactly the same manner, scientists automatically and unconsciously construct emotional meaning from the actions of their lab animals. That’s the mental inference fallacy. It happens every time scientists record a physical measurement and assign it a mental cause. “That growl is expressing anger.” “That change in heartbeat was caused by excitement.” “That brain activity was caused by disgust.” I’m not saying that animal actions are meaningless. I’m saying that it’s a scientific pitfall to presume that an action has emotional meaning.

In the world of animal research, mental inference is rampant. For example, baby rats, when separated from their mother after birth, make a high-pitched noise that sounds to us like crying. Some scientists inferred that the brain circuitry responsible for the crying must be the circuitry for distress. But these baby rats aren’t sad. They’re cold. The sound is just a byproduct as the baby rats try to regulate their body temperature ​— ​a task normally done by their absent mothers. It has nothing to do with emotion. But to an observer, even a well-meaning and highly intelligent one, the sound is easily and automatically perceived as sadness.

Mental inference is normal. Children assign fascinating personalities to their toys. Adults do likewise with their cars. People constantly guess at the meanings of each other’s actions, from raised eyebrows to teenage eye rolls. But scientists in the lab must resist the lure of mental inference, lest they fall prey to the mental inference fallacy and unknowingly taint their research.

To be clear, I’m not saying here that animals cannot experience emotion (a topic that I discuss in much more detail in my book). I’m just saying that mental inference is automatic (in most cultures), and scientists frequently overlook its powerful effects.

So, the next time that you read about emotions being discovered in animals (or in monsters), watch for this pattern. If a scientist labels a action such as freezing using an emotion word like “fear,” you should think, “Aha, the mental inference fallacy strikes again!”

Filed Under: Animals, Emotion, Mental Inference Fallacy, Myths About the Brain, Psychology, Vocabulary

Lisa Feldman Barrett

Primary Sidebar

Seven and a Half Lessons About the Brain

How Emotions are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain

Recent posts

  • The Pufendorf Lectures 2024
  • Rudolf Carnap Lectures 2024
  • Bringing neuroscience to a younger audience
  • A quartet of science questions
  • What I’m Reading Right Now
  • Recommended Books and Videos About the Brain
  • Three Articles on Social Reality
  • Physical Reality Constrains Social Reality, Until it Doesn’t

RSS feed

RSS feed

Archives

  • July 2024
  • April 2023
  • September 2021
  • November 2020
  • October 2020
  • April 2020
  • July 2018
  • January 2018
  • June 2017
  • May 2017
  • April 2017
  • March 2017
  • February 2017
  • January 2017
  • Amazon
  • Bluesky
  • Email
  • Goodreads
  • LinkedIn
  • RSS
  • TikTok
  • Twitter
  • YouTube

Copyright © 2025 · Showcase Pro on Genesis Framework · WordPress · Log in